Skip to main content

Unveiling empathy determinants across borders: a comparative analysis of medical students from two geo-sociocultural backgrounds

Abstract

Background

Empathy plays a pivotal role in healthcare professions, influencing patient satisfaction and treatment outcomes. Understanding the determinants of empathy in medical students is essential. However, findings from previous studies have been inconsistent. We hypothesized that part of this variability may be attributed to the influence of the geo-sociocultural context. In this study, we aimed to compare the longitudinal determinants of empathy in four cohorts of medical students from two distinct geo-sociocultural backgrounds.

Methods

This study included 199 medical students from Porto Alegre, Brazil and Geneva, Switzerland who were in their 1 st and 4 th years of training and agreed to participate on both occasions. The outcome variable was empathy scores (total and subdimensions) assessed through the Jefferson Scale of Empathy for Medical Students (JSE-S). Independent variables included gender, personality, motives for studying medicine, specialty preferences, and the level of motivation for medical studies. Multivariate regression analyses were conducted to investigate the associations between independent variables and empathy at each site in years 1 and 4.

Results

In year 1, the personality trait openness to experience was associated with higher empathy (total score) in both Porto Alegre (b = 0.503, p ≤ 0.01) and Geneva (b = 0.592, p ≤ 0.001), with this association persisting over time. However, the relationships between empathy and the other independent variables varied significantly depending on the site.

Conclusions

The determinants of empathy were significantly modulated by the geo-sociocultural context. The personality trait openness to experience was the only consistent determinant of empathy across both sites over time. Associations between empathy and gender, motives for studying medicine, and specialty preferences were site specific. Our findings underscore the importance of considering students’ backgrounds when assessing empathy teaching and learning.

Peer Review reports

Background

Empathy is a central competence in professions that involve interaction with people, especially patients. A substantial body of evidence sustains that empathy positively impacts the patients’ satisfaction, treatment adherence and clinical outcomes [1, 2]. Therefore, empathy skills are crucial for health professionals and trainees.

In recent years, there has been growing attention to the assessment and training of empathy during medical studies [3, 4]. Various instruments have been used to assess empathy in medical students, including the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [5], the Empathy Quotient (EQ) [6], and the Jefferson Scale of Empathy for medical students (JSE-S) [7], all of which are self-reported measures. The IRI and the EQ were developed for administration to the general population of adults. In contrast, the JSE-S was specifically developed to assess self-reported empathy in medical students and has become the most commonly used tool within the context of medical education [4].

Empathy is an aptitude that may involve both innate personal traits and acquired characteristics influenced by educational and sociocultural backgrounds [8]. Over the years, several studies have used the JSE-S to identify the variables associated with empathic dispositions in medical students, but the results remain unclear. For example, it was often hypothesized that students who have person-oriented motives for studying medicine, such as “altruism” and “caring for patients”, would have higher empathy scores compared to students with security-oriented motives, such as “income” and “prestige”. While some studies support this hypothesis [9, 10], others do not [11, 12]. Similarly, students interested in person-oriented specialties (e.g., pediatrics) as opposed to non-person-oriented specialties (e.g., pathology) have shown higher empathy scores in some studies [9, 13,14,15,16,17] but not in others [18,19,20, 12, 21, 11, 22, 23]. Furthermore, while certain personality traits, such as agreeableness and openness to experience, have been consistently linked to high empathy levels [18, 24,25,26,27], the influence of other traits remains unclear [24, 27, 28].

The discrepancies among studies may be multifactorial [29]. One factor that has been highlighted in recent reports is sociocultural differences. Using the IRI, Chopik et al. found that the association between individual characteristics and self-reported empathy changed depending on the context [30]. Similarly, Zhao et al., using the EQ and the IRI, suggested that the association between female sex and higher self-reported empathy is influenced by cultural context [31], possibly because empathy is more encouraged in women than in men in certain societies. Additionally, a recent review of the literature suggests that changes in empathic dispositions over the course of medical training might be determined by geo-sociocultural characteristics, such as language and whether a culture is more individualistic or collectivistic [32]. However, studies examining the influence of sociocultural context on empathy in medical students remain scarce and inconclusive [4]. Furthermore, no studies have explored the interplay of potential determinants of empathy in a longitudinal cross-national context throughout medical training.

From another perspective, it is recognized that empathy encompasses two constructs embodying emotional and cognitive dimensions [33]. The Jefferson Scale of Empathy for Medical Students (JSE-S) [7] can assess both empathy and its subdomains. However, many studies using the JSE-S have focused solely on global scores, potentially overlooking the emotional and cognitive dimensions. Studies examining the subdimensions of empathy are still lacking.

In this study, we aimed to compare the longitudinal determinants of self-reported empathy in two cohorts of medical students from different sociocultural and educational backgrounds in Brazil and Switzerland. Additionally, we investigated the impact of empathy determinants on the global score as well as the partial subdomains derived from the JSE-S questionnaire.

Methods

Study design

This is a cohort study that started in 2011 at the University of Geneva, Switzerland, and evolved to a larger multicenter project which included among others, the University of Lyon, France, and the Federal University of Health Sciences of Porto Alegre, Brazil. The original research was designed to follow medical students throughout their whole training (6 years) by assessing how contextual and personal characteristics influenced their academic performance, career intentions, and empathy over time [34,35,36]. In each site, two cohorts of students were followed. The selection of students occurred through convenience sampling: all students enrolled in the first year of medical school during the specified academic years were invited to participate. In Geneva, students were recruited during year 1 in 2011 for cohort 1 and 2012 for cohort 2. A total of 767 students were invited to participate, and the response rate was 80%. In Porto Alegre, students were recruited during year 1 in the academic years 2015 (cohort 1) and 2017 (cohort 2). A total of 210 students were invited to participate in Year 1, and the mean response rate was 86.8%. Data collection was performed each year for the purpose of the project, but only data from year 1 and year 4 were considered for the present study.

Participants

Out of 462 students who participated in the study in year 1, and 242 who participated in year 4, 199 students with complete data on both years were included in the final analyses. Of these, 130 were from UNIGE (mean age 19.9 years, 58.5% female) and 69 were from UFCSPA (mean age 20.1 years, 50.7% female).

Data collection was performed through self-reporting questionnaires and, typically, took place in a classroom during school hours. However, because of the Covid- 19 lockdown, data in year 4 at Porto Alegre was collected through online questionnaires.

Social and educational context

The socioeconomic context is different across both study locations; in Geneva the minimum hourly wage it is USD 28.52 [37], while in Porto Alegre it is USD 7.27 [38]. Regarding the medical training in each site, in both Geneva and Porto Alegre there is a predetermined and limited number of admissions for medical student candidates and in both sites the undergraduate medical training lasts six years. However, the selection process and the division of the medical curriculum on preclinical and clinical years between the two sites are different. In Geneva, all students aspiring to be admitted to medical school must undertake a pre-clinical selection year (year 1), where they learn about the fundamentals of medicine. According to their end-of-year academic performance, a subset of students is selected to continue their medical training. Selected students undergo two pre-clinical years (years 2 and 3), two clinical years (years 4 and 5), and one elective year (year 6). By contrast, all medical candidates in Porto Alegre must undertake a national written exam following the conclusion of high school before entering medical school, and those with the higher scores are selected for medical studies. Once admitted, students follow a six-year curriculum track, divided into two pre-clinical years (years 1 and 2), two clinical years (years 3 and 4), and two elective years (years 5 and 6).

Ethical aspects

Before agreeing to take part in the study by signing a consent form, students were informed about the content of the research project, their entitlements and commitments as voluntary participants and the terms of confidentiality and privacy. Participants provided their student ID in order to be matched throughout the duration of the study. Researchers did not have simultaneous access to the data and student IDs, as the latter were managed by a technical administrator. In Geneva, the Chair of the Cantonal Commission for Ethical Research (CCER) designated the study as exempt from formal review. In Porto Alegre, the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Research in Humans from the University of Health Sciences of Porto Alegre (protocol number 1.151.091).

Measures

Empathy

Empathy was assessed using the medical student’s version of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy (JSE-S) validated in French [39] and Portuguese [40]. The JSE-S was specifically developed to measure self-reported empathy in the context of medical education. This tool includes 20 items on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” (1) to “strongly agree” (7). Items 1, 3, 6, 7, 8, 11, 12, 14, 18, 19 are reversely coded. The JSE-S items reflect students’ empathic dispositions, which allow for the calculation of a total score (possible maximum score of 140), as well as three subscores capturing three distinct dimensions, namely Compassionate care, Perspective taking, and Standing in patient’s shoes. As determined by their developers, Compassionate care expresses the emotional domain, while Perspective taking and Standing in patient’s shoes represent the cognitive domain of empathy [33].

Personality

Personality was assessed using the NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) [41] validated to French [42] and Portuguese [43]. The NEO-FFI includes five dimensions: agreeableness, extraversion, openness to experience, conscientiousness, and neuroticism. Each of these dimensions is measured by 12 items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” (0) to “strongly agree” (4). Because previous evidence indicated that personality remains stable over the years [29], the personality questionnaire was applied in year 1 only.

Motives for studying medicine

A list of motives for studying medicine adapted from Vaglum et al. [44] was presented to the students. The following items were included in the survey: income, prestige, liberal activity, saving lives, caring for patients, altruism, mission, vocation, curing illness and academic interest. Students were asked to indicate on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from “not important” to “very important”, how important each of these motives was for themselves. For the statistical analysis, 10 motives were used. On the other hand, for the interpretation of the results, we organized the motives into three categories, also following the work by Vaglum et al.: [44] (1) person-oriented, which includes caring for patients, treating illness, saving lives, mission, vocation and altruism; (2) security-oriented, including prestige, income, and liberal activity; and (3) science-oriented, which includes academic interest.

Specialty preferences

Students were asked to indicate their specialty preference among a list with the following options: internal medicine, family medicine, pediatrics, neurology, rehabilitation, psychiatry, emergency medicine, obstetrics and gynecology, ophthalmology, dermatology, surgery and surgical specialties, radiology, pathology, and anesthesiology. The students could also respond that they were still unsure or undecided. Specialties were then combined into two categories based on the study of Hojat et al. [33]: 1) person-oriented specialties, including internal medicine, family medicine, pediatrics, neurology, rehabilitation, psychiatry, emergency medicine, obstetrics and gynecology, ophthalmology and dermatology; and 2) non person-oriented specialties, which included surgery and surgical specialties, radiology, pathology, and anesthesiology.

Level of motivation for medical studies

Students were asked to indicate how motivated they were for the study of medicine, on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from “very little” (1) to “very much” (6). This was appraised in each survey.

Data analysis

To compare students’ profiles in the two sites, we performed descriptive statistics after stratification by study year and site. Normality of the distribution of the numerical variables was assessed. As they did not appear normal, these numerical variables were tested through Wilcoxon test and expressed as mean ± SD. Categorical variables were tested through Chi-square test and expressed as n (%).

To assess the association of the independent variables with empathy we performed two sets of regressions. Each set included four empathy outcomes (total score and its three subdimension scores). In the first set of regressions, we used only the data collected in year 1. In the second set of regressions, we used data collected in year 4 and added as control variables the scores of empathy subdimensions that were measured in year 1. This enabled us to build models that captured the relationship between the variable measured in both year 1 and year 4, and to assess the associations between the covariates of interest and the dependent variable, while considering the empathy subdimensions measured in year 1 as control variables. Hence, eight regression models were examined in total. In all models, the independent variables considered were site, gender, the five personality traits, the 10 motives for studying medicine, the three specialty preferences (person-oriented, non-person-oriented, undecided), the level of motivation for medical studies as well as interaction terms combining site to each one of the other independent variables. This enabled us to estimate the effect of each variable in each study site. Before the estimation procedure, the Spearman correlation between the independent variables was explored and no excessive correlation was found. Before the estimation procedure, the Spearman correlation between these variables was explored and no excessive correlation was found.

In addition, a thorough residuals’ analysis assessed the adequacy of the model with the data. To interpret the results from the estimated models, we considered the sign, the magnitude, and the significance of the estimated parameters. The level of significance was set at p ≤ 0.05. All statistical analyses were carried out in R (version 4.3.1).

Results

Comparison of students’ profiles between sites

Table 1 details the main characteristics of the 199 students included in the analyses. Students in Porto Alegre had a higher total empathy score than their counterparts from Geneva in year 1 (p < 0.001) and in year 4 (p < 0.001). Further analyses (Tables S- 1 and S- 2 in the Online Supplement) revealed that this was due to Geneva men having lower empathy scores than women, in year 1 (respectively, 109.2 ± 11 vs. 115.7 ± 7.4; p < 0.001) and in year 4 (respectively, 110.5 ± 12.8 vs. 115.4 ± 9.1; p = 0.040). Conversely, in Porto Alegre, there were no significant differences in empathy scores between men and women neither in year 1 (respectively, 117.5 ± 10.9 vs. 118.5 ± 9.9; p = 0.528) or year 4 (respectively, 119.4 ± 10.8 vs. 122 ± 12.2; p = 0.251).

The personality profiles of students from the two sites were significantly different: those in Porto Alegre showed higher scores for neuroticism (p < 0.001) and agreeableness (p = 0.014) while those in Geneva showed more extraversion (p = 0.021) and conscientiousness (p < 0.001). Motives for studying medicine were also different between sites. At year 1, several motives were more relevant to students in Porto Alegre than to those in Geneva: income (p = 0.031), liberal activity (p = 0.015), academic interest (p < 0.001) and saving lives (p = 0.042). However, by year 4, the only significant difference that remained was regarding income as a motive (p < 0.001). The level of motivation for medical studies was similar between the two groups at year 1 (Geneva: 3.7 ± 2.1; Porto Alegre: 3.7 ± 1.4; p = 0.711). Furthermore, motivation increased in both sites overtime, but by year 4 students in Geneva were significantly more motivated than in Porto Alegre (Geneva: 5.0 ± 0.9; Porto Alegre: 4.3 ± 1.4; p < 0.001).

Table 1 Comparison of the main characteristics of the study sample between sites and years

Determinants of students’ empathy at the beginning of medical school (year 1)

Table 2 shows the results of the linear regression analyses investigating—in each study site—the associations between independent variables and empathy scores at year 1.

Table 2 Determinants of empathy in Geneva and Porto Alegre at year 1

In both sites, the total score of empathy was directly associated with the personality trait openness to experience (Geneva: b = 0.592, p ≤ 0.001; Porto Alegre: b = 0.503, p ≤ 0.01). Three additional associations were only found in Geneva: higher empathy scores were associated with having a person-oriented specialty preference (b = 4.324, p ≤ 0.05), while lower empathy scores were associated with being a male (b = − 6.551, p ≤ 0.01) and with having prestige as a motive for studying medicine (b = − 1.849, p ≤ 0.01).

The association between independent variables and the subdomains of empathy were also quite different between sites, except for the direct association found between openness to experience and Perspective taking, which was statistically significant in Geneva (b = 0.277, p ≤ 0.001) and in Porto Alegre (b = 0.391, p ≤ 0.001). All the other significant associations were only found in Geneva.

Determinants of students’ empathy after four years in medical school (year 4)

Table 3 shows the associations between the independent variables and empathy at year 4, after controlling for the empathy subscores at year 1. In other words, the results highlighted in Table 3 present the estimated effects per site of each covariate on the dependent variables measured in year 4, while also incorporating the estimated effects of empathy subscores measured in year 1 on those measured in year 4.

Table 3 Determinants of empathy in Geneva and Porto Alegre at year 4

Regarding the total score of empathy, two associations – which by year 1 were found only in Geneva—appeared also in Porto Alegre at year 4: the direct association with having a person-oriented specialty preference (b = 8.999, p ≤ 0.05) and the inverse association with prestige as a motive for studying medicine (b = − 2.833, p ≤ 0.05). Moreover, several new associations appeared, with notable differences across the two sites. Contrary to the results from the analysis performed considering the dependent variables measured in year 1, the total score of empathy in year 4 was associated with the personality trait agreeableness in Geneva (b = 0.491, p ≤ 0.05), and with the trait extraversion in Porto Alegre (b = 0.519, p ≤ 0.05). Similarly, higher empathy became associated with academic interest as a motive for studying medicine in Geneva (b = 2.501, p ≤ 0.01), and saving lives as a motive in Porto Alegre (b = 5.262, p ≤ 0.01); also, in Porto Alegre alone, lower empathy scores in year 4 were associated with caring for patients as a motive for studying medicine (b = − 3.996, p ≤ 0.05), as opposed to results from year 1 showing no association between these two variables.

In terms of the empathy subdomains, the associations measured in year 4 were not identical across the two sites. For example, in both sites, associations appeared between motives for studying medicine and empathy subdomains, but the specific motives associated with empathy were not the same: in Geneva, academic interest predicted a higher score in both Compassionate care (b = 0.708, p ≤ 0.05) and Perspective taking (b = 1.575, p ≤ 0.01), whereas in Porto Alegre a higher score in Compassionate care was predicted by income as a motive (b = 1.353, p ≤ 0.05), and a higher score in Perspective taking was predicted by saving lives as a motive (b = 3.041, p ≤ 0.01).

Relation between empathy total score at year 4 and its subdomains at year 1

Table 3 shows that the level of empathy at year 1 influenced the level of empathy at year 4, with site-specific differences. In Geneva, both the Compassionate care and the Perspective taking scores at year 1 had a significant estimated effect on empathy in year 4 (b = 0.528, p ≤ 0.05 and b = 0.359, p ≤ 0.05, respectively). By contrast, in Porto Alegre, only the Perspective taking score at year 1 had an effect on empathy in year 4 (b = 0.949, p ≤ 0.001).

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the determinants of empathy in medical students at the start and after four years of training, within two geo-sociocultural contexts. We found that the personality trait openness to experience was associated with higher empathy scores in both sites at both time points. However, other predictors of empathy varied significantly between the two sites, regardless of the year of training. Additionally, we found that empathy levels at year 1 influenced levels at year 4 in both Geneva and Porto Alegre, although not in the same way across the two locations.

The empathy scores of students in both Geneva and Porto Alegre (around 113 and 118 points, respectively) were higher than the minimum empathy scores found in the study by Hojat et al [45]. (95 to 100 points) and were consistent with other results published to date [29]. However, when comparing empathy scores between students from both sites in descriptive analyses, we observed that, in both years, empathy levels in Geneva were significantly lower compared to Porto Alegre. This difference was primarily due to a substantial gender gap in Geneva, where male students scored lower on the JSE scale. Regression analyses further confirmed that being male was specifically associated with lower empathy scores in Geneva for both years. This finding aligns with results reported by Dehning et al [46] who compared medical students between two countries and found that male from Jimma (Ethiopia) exhibited higher empathy levels than those from Munich (Germany), even though female students had higher empathy scores than their male counterparts at both sites. The authors proposed that the lower empathic dispositions among Munich males might be due to cultural norms where emotional behavior and displays of affection among men are traditionally less accepted than in Jimma. Similarly, we believe that cultural norms for men in Brazil and Switzerland differ in ways that could influence the observed results. In Brazil, greater acceptance of emotional expressiveness and interpersonal sensitivity among men may contribute to higher empathic dispositions in Brazilian male medical students compared to their Swiss peers. Furthermore, we consider whether factors beyond the geo-sociocultural context, such as local educational systems or individual experiences, might influence the relationship between gender and empathy, as conflicting results can be found in the literature. For example, while male gender was not associated with lower empathic dispositions in medical students from Porto Alegre, it was in a previous Brazilian study [14]. Similar discrepancy was found in other reports regarding medical students: in Iran, Khademalhosseini et al. [47] found males to have lower empathy than females, while Benabbas et al. [11] did not; and in China, lower empathy in males was found in a study by Wen et al. [48] but not in a study by Li et al. [49] It is important to note that comparing p-values from analyses with different sample sizes requires caution, as significance at one location suggests a gender-based difference, while non-significance elsewhere may simply reflect insufficient sample size. Thus, the association between gender and empathic behaviors in medical students appears to be rather complex and context dependent. More studies are needed to explore interactions between gender and contextual variables on empathic attitudes.

Another potential explanation for the higher empathy levels observed in Brazilian medical students compared to their Swiss peers could be the cultural emphasis on religiosity and spirituality in Brazil. Approximately 95% of the Brazilian people reported adherence to a religion [50], and it is reasonable to assume that the level of religiosity among Brazilian medical students reflects that of the general population. In line with this hypothesis, a recent study conducted in Brazil found that medical students with high levels of spirituality (encompassing both religious and non-religious aspects) scored significantly higher in empathy compared to their peers with lower spirituality levels [51].

The socioeconomic disparity between Porto Alegre and Geneva may also contribute to differences in empathic dispositions among medical students in the two sites. Brazilian students are often more exposed to individuals facing poverty-related health challenges, which may foster a greater sense of compassion and empathy. Although we did not find studies directly linking socioeconomic disparities to empathy, Ponnamperuma et al. [32] reported higher JSE scores among medical students in Brazil (121) compared to those in the USA (115), suggesting that socioeconomic factors could influence empathy levels in the context of medical education.

Regarding the association between personality traits and empathy, we found that openness to experience was the only mutual predictor of empathy in both sites across both years. This aligns with prior research that has consistently linked openness to experience with high empathy as measured not only by the JSE [18, 24,25,26,27] but also by other assessment tools such as the Empathy Quotient [9] and the Interpersonal Reactivity Index [52]. Our study extends this understanding by showing that the impact of openness to experience occurred primarily in Perspective taking, a subdomain linked to the cognitive dimension of empathy. This association was especially evident in year 1 at both sites. The association between openness to experience and Perspective taking may be explained by the fact that, by definition, individuals high in openness to experiencetend to be more adaptable, understanding, and receptive to different ideas and values [53]. Thus, it is reasonable to think that they have an ability to understand and appreciate others’ perspectives—a core element of Perspective taking. However, we noted that, as the training progressed, the relationship between openness to experience and the Perspective taking subdimension in Porto Alegre shifted to an inverse association, which did not occur in Geneva. We speculate whether this change in Porto Alegre could be due to students with high openness to experience becoming more hesitant about adopting empathy-driven behaviors during their clinical years. This hesitation may be attributed to two main factors. First, students in Porto Alegre are likely more exposed to patients suffering during clinical years. These experiencies can be emotionally overwhelming, leading some students to distance themselves as a coping mechanism. Second, the clinical training environment often prioritizes technical skills over emotional skills, which might lead students to perceive empathy as a vulnerability, causing them to suppress empathetic tendencies.

The correlation between specialty preferences and empathy varied depending on the site. In Geneva, students who preferred person-oriented specialties had higher empathy scores in both years. In Porto Alegre, however, the link between specialty preference and empathy depended on the year. While in year 1 there was no association between empathy and specialty preference, by year 4, having any specialty preference (either person- or non-person-oriented) was associated with higher empathy. Previous studies on this topic have produced conflicting results, with some [9, 14, 16, 49] reporting a direct link between preference for person-oriented specialties and empathy, while others did not [18, 19, 11, 12, 20,21,22,23]. While it might seem logical that more empathic students would lean toward specialties involving more patient interaction, as seen in Geneva, research by Guilera et al [9] suggests that students with the highest empathic dispositions sometimes avoid person-oriented specialties to reduce the stress of emotional involvement with patients, which might explain our findings in Porto Alegre.

In our study, the associations between motives for studying medicine and empathic dispositions were also site-dependent. In Geneva, prestige (a security-related motive) was associated with lower empathy in both years, while academic interest (a science-related motive) was associated with higher empathy by year 4. In Porto Alegre, no significant associations were found in year 1, but by year 4, both security- and person-oriented motives were linked to lower empathy. These results suggest that motives for studying medicine were not consistent predictors of empathy in our study. Indeed, previous studies in different countries have shown only weak or no associations between empathy levels and motives for studying medicine [54,55,56]. It is possible that, in places where people face greater economic challenges, like Porto Alegre, even empathic students might prioritize security-related motives for pursuing a medical career.

Our study found that baseline empathy levels in year 1 influenced empathy levels in year 4 in both sites. This suggests that factors affecting empathy when students enter medical school continue to influence their empathic attitudes later on. However, our findings also indicate that there are differences in the way baseline scores of the empathy subdimensions influence future empathic dispositions, varying by geo-sociocultural context. As such, empathy in year 4 was mainly predicted by the year 1 Compassionate care score (emotional component) in Geneva and the Perspective taking score (cognitive component) in Porto Alegre. These differences could be influenced by cultural and societal values. We hypothesize that in Porto Alegre, where there is a strong emphasis on community and collectivism, the emotional component of empathy (Compassionate care) may be already well-developed from the outset, and the cognitive component (Perspective-taking) could become a key differentiating factor in students'empathic dispositions over time. Moreover, in Porto Alegre, Perspective taking scores increased significantly from year 1 to year 4. Conversely, in Geneva, no significant changes in empathy were observed over the course of medical training. This may suggest that training in Porto Alegre might be more effective in reinforcing cognitive empathy compared to Geneva. Alternatively, students in Porto Alegre might be more comfortable developing the cognitive component of empathy as opposed to the emotional one.

Our study has several strengths: (1) it includes students from two very distinct geo-sociocultural contexts but employs the same methodology and research tools for their evaluation, which allows for a more reliable comparative analysis; (2) the use of well-established instruments to measure the outcome and independent variables, which increases credibility and reliability of the data; and (3) the use of multivariate analyses, which allows for controlling the effect of confounding variables. Despite these strengths, the study has some limitations. First, only about 50% of the students who were initially enrolled in year 1 participated in year 4, and this could impact the representativeness of the data. Second, the sample size in Geneva was larger than in Porto Alegre, and this could also influence the representation of the respective populations. However, we observed low standard deviations in empathy scores across both locations and years. This suggests that, despite the differences in participation rates and sample sizes, the empathy scores were consistent and similar, indicating that the samples were homogeneous and can still be considered representative of the study populations. Another limitation of our study was the relative discrepancy in the timing and method of data collection between the two study sites. In Geneva, students were recruited in 2011–2012, while in Porto Alegre, data were collected later, in 2015–2017. Additionally, part of the Brazilian data was collected during the COVID- 19 pandemic using online questionnaires, whereas all Swiss data were collected before the pandemic using physical questionnaires. It is possible that the method of data collection, as well as heightened social awareness during the covid pandemic, may have influenced empathic behaviors or the way it was reported by the participants of the study.

Conclusions

Our study provides evidence that the determinants of empathy, including personality traits, gender, motives for studying medicine and specialty preferences on self-reported empathy – either the total scores or the subdomains – differ according to the geo-sociocultural context, with openness to experience being the sole consistent predictor of empathy across sites. To the best of our knowledge, this is the only study addressing an expressive number of relevant individual characteristics to assess the determinants of empathic dispositions, which may impact on the development of this key ability for clinical practice during medical studies. Our findings underscore the importance of the cultural context for designing medical education interventions to improve students’ empathic attitudes. Tailored training approaches, considering the unique socio-cultural characteristics of each location, are crucial for enhancing empathic dispositions and attitudes in future health professionals.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Larson EB, Yao X. Clinical empathy as emotional labor in the patient-physician relationship. JAMA. 2005;293(9):1100–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Kim SS, Kaplowitz S, Johnston MV. The effects of physician empathy on patient satisfaction and compliance. Eval Health Prof. 2004;27(3):237–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Batt-Rawden SA, Chisolm MS, Anton B, Flickinger TE. Teaching empathy to medical students: an updated, systematic review. Acad Med. 2013;88(8):1171–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Andersen FA, Johansen ASB, Søndergaard J, Andersen CM, Assing HE. Revisiting the trajectory of medical students’ empathy, and impact of gender, specialty preferences and nationality: a systematic review. BMC Med Educ. 2020;20(1):52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JASAS Catalog Selected Doc Psychol. 1980;10:85–104.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(2):163–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Hojat M, DeSantis J, Shannon SC, Mortensen LH, Speicher MR, Bragan L, et al. The Jefferson Scale of Empathy: a nationwide study of measurement properties, underlying components, latent variable structure, and national norms in medical students. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2018;23(5):899–920.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Hojat M. Descriptions and Conceptualization. In: Hojat M, editor. Empathy in Health Professions Education and Patient Care [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2016 [cited 2023 Sep 11]. p. 3–16. Available from: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/978-3-319-27625-0_1.

  9. Guilera T, Batalla I, Forné C, Soler-González J. Empathy and big five personality model in medical students and its relationship to gender and specialty preference: a cross-sectional study. BMC Med Educ. 2019;19(1):57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Piumatti G, Abbiati M, Baroffio A, Gerbase MW. Associations between motivational factors for studying medicine, learning approaches and empathy among medical school candidates. Adv in Health Sci Educ. 2019;24(2):287–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Benabbas R. Empathy in Iranian medical students: A comparison by age, gender, academic performance and specialty preferences. Med J Islam Repub Iran. 2016;30:439.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Shashikumar R, Chaudhary R, Ryali VSSR, Bhat PS, Srivastava K, Prakash J, et al. Cross sectional assessment of empathy among undergraduates from a medical college. Med J Armed Forces India. 2014;70(2):179–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Hojat M, Zuckerman M, Magee M, Mangione S, Nasca T, Vergare M, et al. Empathy in medical students as related to specialty interest, personality, and perceptions of mother and father. Personality Individ Differ. 2005;39:1205–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Santos MA, Grosseman S, Morelli TC, Giuliano ICB, Erdmann TR. Empathy differences by gender and specialty preference in medical students: a study in Brazil. Int J Med Educ. 2016;7:149–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Chen DCR, Kirshenbaum DS, Yan J, Kirshenbaum E, Aseltine RH. Characterizing changes in student empathy throughout medical school. Med Teach. 2012;34(4):305–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Tavakol S, Dennick R, Tavakol M. Empathy in UK medical students: differences by gender, medical year and specialty interest. Educ Prim Care. 2011;22(5):297–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Chen D, Lew R, Hershman W, Orlander J. A cross-sectional measurement of medical student empathy. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(10):1434–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. O’Tuathaigh CMP, Nadhirah Idris A, Duggan E, Costa P, Costa MJ. Medical students’ empathy and attitudes towards professionalism: Relationship with personality, specialty preference and medical programme. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(5): e0215675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Tariq N, Rasheed T, Tavakol M. A Quantitative Study of Empathy in Pakistani Medical Students: A Multicentered Approach. J Prim Care Community Health. 2017;8(4):294–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Youssef FF, Nunes P, Sa B, Williams S. An exploration of changes in cognitive and emotional empathy among medical students in the Caribbean. Int J Med Educ. 2014;5:185–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Magalhães E, Salgueira AP, Costa P, Costa MJ. Empathy in senior year and first year medical students: a cross-sectional study. BMC Med Educ. 2011;11(1):52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Akgün Ö, Akdeniz M, Kavukcu E, Avcı HH. Medical Students’ Empathy Level Differences by Medical Year, Gender, and Specialty Interest in Akdeniz University. J Med Educ Curric Dev. 2020;7:2382120520940658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Hasan S, Al-Sharqawi N, Dashti F, AbdulAziz M, Abdullah A, Shukkur M, et al. Level of empathy among medical students in Kuwait University. Kuwait Med Princ Pract. 2013;22(4):385–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Abe K, Niwa M, Fujisaki K, Suzuki Y. Associations between emotional intelligence, empathy and personality in Japanese medical students. BMC Med Educ. 2018;18(1):47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Costa P, Magalhães E, Costa MJ. A latent growth model suggests that empathy of medical students does not decline over time. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2013;18(3):509–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Costa P, Alves R, Neto I, Marvão P, Portela M, Costa MJ. Associations between medical student empathy and personality: a multi-institutional study. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(3): e89254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Magalhães E, Costa P, Costa MJ. Empathy of medical students and personality: evidence from the Five-Factor Model. Med Teach. 2012;34(10):807–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Romosan RS, Dehelean L, Enatescu VR, Bredicean AC, Papava I, Giurgi-Oncu C, et al. Profiling undergraduate students from a Romanian medical university. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:1891–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Quince T, Thiemann P, Benson J, Hyde S. Undergraduate medical students’ empathy: current perspectives. Adv Med Educ Pract. 2016;7:443–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Chopik WJ, O’Brien E, Konrath SH. Differences in empathic concern and perspective taking across 63 countries. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2017;48(1):23–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Zhao Q, Neumann DL, Cao Y, Baron-Cohen S, Yan C, Chan RCK, et al. Culture-Sex Interaction and the Self-Report Empathy in Australians and Mainland Chinese. Front Psychol. 2019;10:396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ponnamperuma G, Yeo SP, Samarasekera DD. Is empathy change in medical school geo-socioculturally influenced? Med Educ. 2019;53(7):655–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Nasca TJ, Mangione S, Vergare M, Magee M. Physician empathy: definition, components, measurement, and relationship to gender and specialty. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(9):1563–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Pfarrwaller E, Voirol L, Piumatti G, Karemera M, Sommer J, Gerbase MW, et al. Students’ intentions to practice primary care are associated with their motives to become doctors: a longitudinal study. BMC Med Educ. 2022;22(1):30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Piumatti G, Abbiati M, Gerbase MW, Baroffio A. Patterns of Change in Approaches to Learning and Their Impact on Academic Performance Among Medical Students: Longitudinal Analysis. Teach Learn Med. 2021;33(2):173–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Piumatti G, Abbiati M, Baroffio A, Gerbase MW. Empathy trajectories throughout medical school: relationships with personality and motives for studying medicine. Adv in Health Sci Educ. 2020;25(5):1227–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Confédération Suisse. Minimum wage in the canton of Geneva [Internet]. [cited 2024 Jan 13]. Available from: https://www.eda.admin.ch/missions/mission-onu-geneve/en/home/manual-regime-privileges-and-immunities/introduction/manual-labour-law/Salaire-minimum-dans-le-canton-de-Geneve.html.

  38. Brasil. Decreto no 11.864, de 27 de dezembro de 2023 [Internet]. Diário Oficial da União. [cited 2024 Jan 13]. Available from: https://www.in.gov.br/web/dou.

  39. Gerbase MW, Gustin MP, Bajwa N, Abbiati M, Baroffio A. Development and Cross-National Validation of a French Version of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy for Students. Eval Health Prof. 2022;45(3):288–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Paro HB, Daud-Gallotti RM, Tibério IC, Pinto RM, Martins MA. Brazilian version of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy: psychometric properties and factor analysis. BMC Med Educ. 2012;12:73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. J Pers. 1992;60(2):175–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Aluja A, Garcı́a O, Rossier J, Garcı́a LF. Comparison of the NEO-FFI, the NEO-FFI-R and an alternative short version of the NEO-PI-R (NEO-60) in Swiss and Spanish samples. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;38(3):591–604.

  43. Magalhães E, Salgueira A, Gonzalez AJ, Costa JJ, Costa MJ, Costa P, et al. NEO-FFI: Psychometric properties of a short personality inventory in Portuguese context. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2014;27(4):642–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Vaglum P, Wiers-Jenssen J, Ekeberg O. Motivation for medical school: the relationship to gender and specialty preferences in a nationwide sample. Med Educ. 1999;33(4):236–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hojat M, Gonnella JS. Eleven Years of Data on the Jefferson Scale of Empathy-Medical Student Version (JSE-S): Proxy Norm Data and Tentative Cutoff Scores. Med Princ Pract. 2015;24(4):344–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Dehning S, Gasperi S, Tesfaye M, Girma E, Meyer S, Krahl W, et al. Empathy Without Borders? Cross-Cultural Heart and Mind-Reading in First-Year Medical Students. Ethiop J Health Sci. 2013;23(2):113–22.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Khademalhosseini M, Khademalhosseini Z, Mahmoodian F. Comparison of empathy score among medical students in both basic and clinical levels. J Adv Med Educ Prof. 2014;2(2):88–91.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Wen D, Ma X, Li H, Liu Z, Xian B, Liu Y. Empathy in Chinese medical students: psychometric characteristics and differences by gender and year of medical education. BMC Med Educ. 2013;13:130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Li D, Xu H, Kang M, Ma S. Empathy in Chinese eight-year medical program students: differences by school year, educational stage, and future career preference. BMC Med Educ. 2018;18(1):241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Censo demográfico 2010: características gerais da população, religião e pessoas com deficiência. [Internet]. 2012 [cited 2025 Jan 26]. Available from: https://biblioteca.ibge.gov.br/visualizacao/periodicos/94/cd_2010_religiao_deficiencia.pdf.

  51. Moura AT, Coriolano AM, Kobayasi R, Pessanha S, Cruz HL, Melo SM, et al. Is there an association among spirituality, resilience and empathy in medical students? BMC Med Educ. 2024;24:704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Lourinho I, Severo M. Are personality traits really weak/moderate predictors of empathy? Med Teach. 2013;35(7):611–611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychol Assess. 1992;4(1):5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Gonçalves-Pereira M, Trancas B, Loureiro J, Papoila A, Caldas-de-Almeida JM. Empathy as related to motivations for medicine in a sample of first-year medical students. Psychol Rep. 2013;112(1):73–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. McManus IC, Livingston G, Katona C. The attractions of medicine: the generic motivations of medical school applicants in relation to demography, personality and achievement. BMC Med Educ. 2006;6:11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Loureiro J, Gonçalves-Pereira M, Trancas B, Caldas-de-Almeida JM, Castro-Caldas A. Empathy in the doctor-patient relationship as viewed by first-year medical students: data on validity and sensibility to change of the Jefferson Measure in Portugal. Acta Med Port. 2011;24(Suppl 2):431–42.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Prof. Mohammadreza Hojat for the kind permission to use the Jefferson Scale of Empathy for Medical Students. They acknowledge the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio Grande do Sul (FAPERGS), in Porto Alegre, Brazil; the Swiss Federal Office of Public Health, the Faculty of Medicine at the University of Geneva and the General Fund of the University of Geneva (Maus Foundation), in Geneva, Switzerland for the financial support of the study. They are also grateful to the students who participated in the study.

Funding

Open access funding provided by University of Geneva This work was partially funded by the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio Grande do Sul (FAPERGS), in Porto Alegre, Brazil; and by the Swiss Federal Office of Public Health, the Faculty of Medicine at the University of Geneva, and the General Fund of the University of Geneva (Maus Foundation), in Geneva, Switzerland.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

FB participated in the collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, and wrote the manuscript. LV analyzed and interpreted the data, contributing to the writing and revision of the manuscript. SG analyzed and interpreted the data as the senior statistician, also contributing to the writing and revision of the manuscript. MK contributed in the analyses and interpretation of data, and the revision of the manuscript. RF contributed in the analyses, interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript. AB and MWG conceived and coordinated the study, wrote the protocol and participated in the collection of data, analysis and interpretation of results, as well as contributed to the writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Margaret W. Gerbase.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study project was submitted to the Chair of the Geneva Cantonal Commission for Ethical Research (CCER) and designated as exempt from formal review. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Research in Humans from the Federal University of Health Sciences of Porto Alegre (protocol number 1.151.091). Informed consent was obtained from all subjects participating in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Balbinot, F., Voirol, L., Guerrier, S. et al. Unveiling empathy determinants across borders: a comparative analysis of medical students from two geo-sociocultural backgrounds. BMC Med Educ 25, 554 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12909-025-07109-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12909-025-07109-7

Keywords